From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia

Article Draft

Lead

Affective neuroscience is the study of the how the brain processes emotions. This field combines neuroscience with the psychological study of personality, emotion, and mood [1]. The basis of emotions and what emotions are remains an issue of debate within the field of affective neuroscience [2].

The term "affective neuroscience" was coined by neuroscientist Jaak Panksepp, at a time when cognitive neuroscience focused on parts of psychology that did not include emotion, such as attention or memory [3].

Article body

Cognitive neuroscience tasks in affective neuroscience research

Ekman 60 faces task

The Ekman faces task is used to measure emotion recognition of six basic emotions. [4] [5] Black and white photographs of 10 actors (6 male, 4 female) are presented, with each actor displaying each emotion. Participants are usually asked to respond quickly with the name of the displayed emotion. The task is a common tool to study deficits in emotion regulation in patients with dementia, Parkinson's, and other cognitively degenerative disorders. [6] The task has been used to analyze recognition errors in disorders such as borderline personality disorder, schizophrenia, and bipolar disorder. [7] [8] [9]

Dot probe (emotion)

The emotional dot-probe paradigm is a task used to assess selective visual attention to and failure to detach attention from affective stimuli. [10] [11] The paradigm begins with a fixation cross at the center of a screen. An emotional stimulus and a neutral stimulus appear side by side, after which a dot appears behind either the neutral stimulus (incongruent condition) or the affective stimulus (congruent condition). Participants are asked to indicate when they see this dot, and response latency is measured. Dots that appear on the same side of the screen as the image the participant was looking at will be identified more quickly. Thus, it is possible to discern which object the participant was attending to by subtracting the reaction time to respond to congruent versus incongruent trials. [10]

The best documented research with the dot probe paradigm involves attention to threat related stimuli, such as fearful faces, in individuals with anxiety disorders. Anxious individuals tend to respond more quickly to congruent trials, which may indicate vigilance to threat and/or failure to detach attention from threatening stimuli. [10] [12] A specificity effect of attention has also been noted, with individuals attending selectively to threats related to their particular disorder. For example, those with social phobia selectively attend to social threats but not physical threats. [13] However, this specificity may be even more nuanced. Participants with obsessive-compulsive disorder symptoms initially show attentional bias to compulsive threat, but this bias is attenuated in later trials due to habituation to the threat stimuli. [14]

Fear potentiated startle

Fear-potentiated startle (FPS) has been utilized as a psychophysiological index of fear reaction in both animals and humans. [15] FPS is most often assessed through the magnitude of the eyeblink startle reflex, which can be measured by electromyography. [16] This eyeblink reflex is an automatic defensive reaction to an abrupt elicitor, making it an objective indicator of fear. [17] Typical FPS paradigms involve bursts of noise or abrupt flashes of light transmitted while an individual attends to a set of stimuli. [17] Startle reflexes have been shown to be modulated by emotion. For example, healthy participants tend to show enhanced startle responses while viewing negatively valenced images and attenuated startle while viewing positively valenced images, as compared with neutral images. [18] [19]

The startle response to a particular stimulus is greater under conditions of threat. [20] A common example given to indicate this phenomenon is that one's startle response to a flash of light will be greater when walking in a dangerous neighborhood at night than it would under safer conditions. In laboratory studies, the threat of receiving shock is enough to potentiate startle, even without any actual shock. [21]

Fear potentiated startle paradigms are often used to study fear learning and extinction in individuals with post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) and other anxiety disorders. [22] [23] [24] In fear conditioning studies, an initially neutral stimulus is repeatedly paired with an aversive one, borrowing from classical conditioning. [25] FPS studies have demonstrated that PTSD patients have enhanced startle responses during both danger cues and neutral/safety cues as compared with healthy participants. [25] [26]


References

  1. ^ Panksepp, Jaak (1990), "A Role for Affective Neuroscience in Understanding Stress: The Case of Separation Distress Circuitry", Psychobiology of Stress, Dordrecht: Springer Netherlands, pp. 41–57, ISBN  978-94-010-7390-5, retrieved 2022-11-27
  2. ^ Celeghin, Alessia; Diano, Matteo; Bagnis, Arianna; Viola, Marco; Tamietto, Marco (2017-08-24). "Basic Emotions in Human Neuroscience: Neuroimaging and Beyond". Frontiers in Psychology. 8. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01432. ISSN  1664-1078.{{ cite journal}}: CS1 maint: unflagged free DOI ( link)
  3. ^ Panksepp, Jaak (2004). Affective Neuroscience: The Foundations of Human and Animal Emotions (1st ed.). Oxford University Press. ISBN  978-0195178050.
  4. ^ Diehl-Schmid, J.; Pohl, C.; Ruprecht, C.; Wagenpfeil, S.; Foerstl, H.; Kurz, A. (2007). "The Ekman 60 faces test as a diagnostic instrument in frontotemporal dementia". Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology. 22 (4): 459–464. doi: 10.1016/j.acn.2007.01.024. PMID  17360152.
  5. ^ Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. (1976). Pictures of facial affect. Consulting Psychologists Press: Palo Alto, CA.
  6. ^ Diehl-Schmida, J.; Pohla, C.; Ruprechta, C.; Wagenpfeilb, S.; Foerstla, H.; Kurza, A. (2007). "The Ekman 60 faces test as a diagnostic instrument in frontotemporal dementia". Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology. 22 (4): 459–464. doi: 10.1016/j.acn.2007.01.024. PMID  17360152.
  7. ^ Ibarretxe-Bilbao, N.; Junque, C.; Tolosa, E.; Marti, M.; Valldeoriola, F.; Bargalloand, N.; Zareio, M. (2009). "Neuroanatomical correlates of impaired decision-making and facial emotion recognition in early Parkinson's disease". European Journal of Neuroscience. 30 (6): 1162–1171. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2009.06892.x. PMID  19735293. S2CID  413809.
  8. ^ Soeiro-de-Souza, M.G.; Bio, D.S.; David, D.P.; Santos, D.R.; Kerr, D.S.; Moreno, R.A.; Machado-Vieira, Rodrigo; Moreno, Ricardo Albeto (2012). "COMT Met (158) modulates facial emotion recognition in bipolar I disorder mood episodes". Journal of Affective Disorders. 136 (3): 370–376. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2011.11.021. PMID  22222175.
  9. ^ Ebert, A.; Haussleiter, I.S.; Juckel, G.; Brune, M.; Roser, P. (2012). "Impaired facial emotion recognition in a ketamine model of psychosis". Psychiatry Research. 200 (2–3): 724–727. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2012.06.034. PMID  22776754. S2CID  43744551.
  10. ^ a b c Koster, E.H.W.; Crombez, G.; Verschuere, B.; De Houwer, J. (2004). "Selective attention to threat in the dot probe paradigm: Differentiating vigilance and difficulty to disengage". Behaviour Research and Therapy. 42 (10): 1183–1192. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2003.08.001. PMID  15350857.
  11. ^ MacLeod, C.; Mathews, A.; Tata, P. (1986). "Attentional bias in emotional disorders". Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 95 (1): 15–20. doi: 10.1037/0021-843x.95.1.15. PMID  3700842.
  12. ^ Mogg, K.; Bradley, B. P. (1998). "A cognitive–motivational analysis of anxiety". Behaviour Research and Therapy. 36 (9): 809–848. doi: 10.1016/s0005-7967(98)00063-1. PMID  9701859.
  13. ^ Asmundson, G.J.G.; Stein, M.B. (1994). "Selective processing of social threat in patients with generalized social phobia: Evaluation using a dot-probe paradigm". Journal of Anxiety Disorders. 8 (2): 107–117. doi: 10.1016/0887-6185(94)90009-4.
  14. ^ Amir, N.; Naimi, S.; Morrison, A.S. (2009). "Attenuation of attention bias in obsessive-compulsive disorder". Behaviour Research and Therapy. 47 (2): 153–157. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2008.10.020. PMC  2662360. PMID  19046576.
  15. ^ Davis, M. (1986). "Pharmacological and anatomical analysis of fear conditioning using the fear-potentiated startle paradigm". Behavioral Neuroscience. 100 (6): 814–824. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.100.6.814. PMID  3545257.
  16. ^ Baskin-Sommers, A.R.; Vitale, J.E.; MacCoon, D.; Newman, J.P. (2012). "Assessing emotion sensitivity in female offenders with borderline personality symptoms: Results from a fear-potentiated startle paradigm". Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 121 (2): 477–483. doi: 10.1037/a0026753. PMC  3358451. PMID  22250659.
  17. ^ a b Vaidyanathan, U.; Patrick, C.J.; Cuthbert, B.N. (2009). "Linking dimensional models of internalizing psychopathology to neurobiological systems: Affect-modulated startle as an indicator of fear and distress disorders and affiliated traits". Psychological Bulletin. 135 (6): 909–942. doi: 10.1037/a0017222. PMC  2776729. PMID  19883142.
  18. ^ Lang, P. J.; Bradley, M. M.; Cuthbert, B. N. (1990). "Emotion, attention, and the startle reflex". Psychological Review. 97 (3): 377–395. doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.97.3.377. PMID  2200076.
  19. ^ Vrana, S. R.; Spence, E. L.; Lang, P. J. (1988). "The startle probe response: A new measure of emotion?". Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 97 (4): 487–491. doi: 10.1037/0021-843x.97.4.487. PMID  3204235.
  20. ^ Norrholm, S.D.; Anderson, K.M.; Olin, I.W.; Jovanovic, T.; Kwon, C.; Warren, Victor T.; Bradley, B.; Bosshardt, Lauren; Sabree, Justin; Duncan, Erica J.; Rothbaum, Barbara O.; Bradley, Bekh (2011). "Versatility of fear-potentiated startle paradigms for assessing human conditioned fear extinction and return of fear". Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience. 5: 77. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2011.00077. PMC  3221285. PMID  22125516.
  21. ^ Grillon, C.; Ameli, R.; Woods, S. W.; Merikangas, K.; Davis, M. (1991). "Fear-potentiated startle in humans: Effects of anticipatory anxiety on the acoustic blink reflex". Psychophysiology. 28 (5): 588–595. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1991.tb01999.x. PMID  1758934.
  22. ^ Norrholm, S.; Jovanovic, Tanja (2011). "Translational fear inhibition models as indices of trauma-related psychopathology". Current Psychiatry Reviews. 7 (3): 194–204. doi: 10.2174/157340011797183193.
  23. ^ Norrholm, S. D.; Jovanovic, T.; Olin, I. W.; Sands, L.; Karapanou, I.; Bradley, B.; Ressler, K. J. (2010). "Fear extinction in traumatized civilians with posttraumatic stress disorder: relation to symptom severity". Biological Psychiatry. 69 (6): 556–563. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.09.013. PMC  3052965. PMID  21035787.
  24. ^ Lang, P. J.; McTeague, L. M. (2009). "The anxiety disorder spectrum: Fear imagery, physiological reactivity, and differential diagnosis". Anxiety, Stress & Coping. 22 (1): 5–25. doi: 10.1080/10615800802478247. PMC  2766521. PMID  19096959.
  25. ^ a b Cousens, G.A.; Kearns, A.; Laterza, F.; Tundidor, J. (2012). "Excitotoxic lesions of the medial amygdala attenuate olfactory fear-potentiated startle and conditioned freezing behavior". Behavioural Brain Research. 229 (2): 427–432. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2012.01.011. PMID  22249137. S2CID  25116098.
  26. ^ Jovanovic, T; Norrholm, Seth D.; Fennell, Jennifer E.; Keyes, Megan; Fiallos, Ana M.; Myers, Karyn M.; Davis, Michael; Duncan, Erica J. (15 May 2009). "Posttraumatic stress disorder may be associated with impaired fear inhibition: relation to symptom severity". Psychiatry Research. 167 (1–2): 151–160. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2007.12.014. PMC  2713500. PMID  19345420.
From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia

Article Draft

Lead

Affective neuroscience is the study of the how the brain processes emotions. This field combines neuroscience with the psychological study of personality, emotion, and mood [1]. The basis of emotions and what emotions are remains an issue of debate within the field of affective neuroscience [2].

The term "affective neuroscience" was coined by neuroscientist Jaak Panksepp, at a time when cognitive neuroscience focused on parts of psychology that did not include emotion, such as attention or memory [3].

Article body

Cognitive neuroscience tasks in affective neuroscience research

Ekman 60 faces task

The Ekman faces task is used to measure emotion recognition of six basic emotions. [4] [5] Black and white photographs of 10 actors (6 male, 4 female) are presented, with each actor displaying each emotion. Participants are usually asked to respond quickly with the name of the displayed emotion. The task is a common tool to study deficits in emotion regulation in patients with dementia, Parkinson's, and other cognitively degenerative disorders. [6] The task has been used to analyze recognition errors in disorders such as borderline personality disorder, schizophrenia, and bipolar disorder. [7] [8] [9]

Dot probe (emotion)

The emotional dot-probe paradigm is a task used to assess selective visual attention to and failure to detach attention from affective stimuli. [10] [11] The paradigm begins with a fixation cross at the center of a screen. An emotional stimulus and a neutral stimulus appear side by side, after which a dot appears behind either the neutral stimulus (incongruent condition) or the affective stimulus (congruent condition). Participants are asked to indicate when they see this dot, and response latency is measured. Dots that appear on the same side of the screen as the image the participant was looking at will be identified more quickly. Thus, it is possible to discern which object the participant was attending to by subtracting the reaction time to respond to congruent versus incongruent trials. [10]

The best documented research with the dot probe paradigm involves attention to threat related stimuli, such as fearful faces, in individuals with anxiety disorders. Anxious individuals tend to respond more quickly to congruent trials, which may indicate vigilance to threat and/or failure to detach attention from threatening stimuli. [10] [12] A specificity effect of attention has also been noted, with individuals attending selectively to threats related to their particular disorder. For example, those with social phobia selectively attend to social threats but not physical threats. [13] However, this specificity may be even more nuanced. Participants with obsessive-compulsive disorder symptoms initially show attentional bias to compulsive threat, but this bias is attenuated in later trials due to habituation to the threat stimuli. [14]

Fear potentiated startle

Fear-potentiated startle (FPS) has been utilized as a psychophysiological index of fear reaction in both animals and humans. [15] FPS is most often assessed through the magnitude of the eyeblink startle reflex, which can be measured by electromyography. [16] This eyeblink reflex is an automatic defensive reaction to an abrupt elicitor, making it an objective indicator of fear. [17] Typical FPS paradigms involve bursts of noise or abrupt flashes of light transmitted while an individual attends to a set of stimuli. [17] Startle reflexes have been shown to be modulated by emotion. For example, healthy participants tend to show enhanced startle responses while viewing negatively valenced images and attenuated startle while viewing positively valenced images, as compared with neutral images. [18] [19]

The startle response to a particular stimulus is greater under conditions of threat. [20] A common example given to indicate this phenomenon is that one's startle response to a flash of light will be greater when walking in a dangerous neighborhood at night than it would under safer conditions. In laboratory studies, the threat of receiving shock is enough to potentiate startle, even without any actual shock. [21]

Fear potentiated startle paradigms are often used to study fear learning and extinction in individuals with post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) and other anxiety disorders. [22] [23] [24] In fear conditioning studies, an initially neutral stimulus is repeatedly paired with an aversive one, borrowing from classical conditioning. [25] FPS studies have demonstrated that PTSD patients have enhanced startle responses during both danger cues and neutral/safety cues as compared with healthy participants. [25] [26]


References

  1. ^ Panksepp, Jaak (1990), "A Role for Affective Neuroscience in Understanding Stress: The Case of Separation Distress Circuitry", Psychobiology of Stress, Dordrecht: Springer Netherlands, pp. 41–57, ISBN  978-94-010-7390-5, retrieved 2022-11-27
  2. ^ Celeghin, Alessia; Diano, Matteo; Bagnis, Arianna; Viola, Marco; Tamietto, Marco (2017-08-24). "Basic Emotions in Human Neuroscience: Neuroimaging and Beyond". Frontiers in Psychology. 8. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01432. ISSN  1664-1078.{{ cite journal}}: CS1 maint: unflagged free DOI ( link)
  3. ^ Panksepp, Jaak (2004). Affective Neuroscience: The Foundations of Human and Animal Emotions (1st ed.). Oxford University Press. ISBN  978-0195178050.
  4. ^ Diehl-Schmid, J.; Pohl, C.; Ruprecht, C.; Wagenpfeil, S.; Foerstl, H.; Kurz, A. (2007). "The Ekman 60 faces test as a diagnostic instrument in frontotemporal dementia". Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology. 22 (4): 459–464. doi: 10.1016/j.acn.2007.01.024. PMID  17360152.
  5. ^ Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. (1976). Pictures of facial affect. Consulting Psychologists Press: Palo Alto, CA.
  6. ^ Diehl-Schmida, J.; Pohla, C.; Ruprechta, C.; Wagenpfeilb, S.; Foerstla, H.; Kurza, A. (2007). "The Ekman 60 faces test as a diagnostic instrument in frontotemporal dementia". Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology. 22 (4): 459–464. doi: 10.1016/j.acn.2007.01.024. PMID  17360152.
  7. ^ Ibarretxe-Bilbao, N.; Junque, C.; Tolosa, E.; Marti, M.; Valldeoriola, F.; Bargalloand, N.; Zareio, M. (2009). "Neuroanatomical correlates of impaired decision-making and facial emotion recognition in early Parkinson's disease". European Journal of Neuroscience. 30 (6): 1162–1171. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2009.06892.x. PMID  19735293. S2CID  413809.
  8. ^ Soeiro-de-Souza, M.G.; Bio, D.S.; David, D.P.; Santos, D.R.; Kerr, D.S.; Moreno, R.A.; Machado-Vieira, Rodrigo; Moreno, Ricardo Albeto (2012). "COMT Met (158) modulates facial emotion recognition in bipolar I disorder mood episodes". Journal of Affective Disorders. 136 (3): 370–376. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2011.11.021. PMID  22222175.
  9. ^ Ebert, A.; Haussleiter, I.S.; Juckel, G.; Brune, M.; Roser, P. (2012). "Impaired facial emotion recognition in a ketamine model of psychosis". Psychiatry Research. 200 (2–3): 724–727. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2012.06.034. PMID  22776754. S2CID  43744551.
  10. ^ a b c Koster, E.H.W.; Crombez, G.; Verschuere, B.; De Houwer, J. (2004). "Selective attention to threat in the dot probe paradigm: Differentiating vigilance and difficulty to disengage". Behaviour Research and Therapy. 42 (10): 1183–1192. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2003.08.001. PMID  15350857.
  11. ^ MacLeod, C.; Mathews, A.; Tata, P. (1986). "Attentional bias in emotional disorders". Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 95 (1): 15–20. doi: 10.1037/0021-843x.95.1.15. PMID  3700842.
  12. ^ Mogg, K.; Bradley, B. P. (1998). "A cognitive–motivational analysis of anxiety". Behaviour Research and Therapy. 36 (9): 809–848. doi: 10.1016/s0005-7967(98)00063-1. PMID  9701859.
  13. ^ Asmundson, G.J.G.; Stein, M.B. (1994). "Selective processing of social threat in patients with generalized social phobia: Evaluation using a dot-probe paradigm". Journal of Anxiety Disorders. 8 (2): 107–117. doi: 10.1016/0887-6185(94)90009-4.
  14. ^ Amir, N.; Naimi, S.; Morrison, A.S. (2009). "Attenuation of attention bias in obsessive-compulsive disorder". Behaviour Research and Therapy. 47 (2): 153–157. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2008.10.020. PMC  2662360. PMID  19046576.
  15. ^ Davis, M. (1986). "Pharmacological and anatomical analysis of fear conditioning using the fear-potentiated startle paradigm". Behavioral Neuroscience. 100 (6): 814–824. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.100.6.814. PMID  3545257.
  16. ^ Baskin-Sommers, A.R.; Vitale, J.E.; MacCoon, D.; Newman, J.P. (2012). "Assessing emotion sensitivity in female offenders with borderline personality symptoms: Results from a fear-potentiated startle paradigm". Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 121 (2): 477–483. doi: 10.1037/a0026753. PMC  3358451. PMID  22250659.
  17. ^ a b Vaidyanathan, U.; Patrick, C.J.; Cuthbert, B.N. (2009). "Linking dimensional models of internalizing psychopathology to neurobiological systems: Affect-modulated startle as an indicator of fear and distress disorders and affiliated traits". Psychological Bulletin. 135 (6): 909–942. doi: 10.1037/a0017222. PMC  2776729. PMID  19883142.
  18. ^ Lang, P. J.; Bradley, M. M.; Cuthbert, B. N. (1990). "Emotion, attention, and the startle reflex". Psychological Review. 97 (3): 377–395. doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.97.3.377. PMID  2200076.
  19. ^ Vrana, S. R.; Spence, E. L.; Lang, P. J. (1988). "The startle probe response: A new measure of emotion?". Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 97 (4): 487–491. doi: 10.1037/0021-843x.97.4.487. PMID  3204235.
  20. ^ Norrholm, S.D.; Anderson, K.M.; Olin, I.W.; Jovanovic, T.; Kwon, C.; Warren, Victor T.; Bradley, B.; Bosshardt, Lauren; Sabree, Justin; Duncan, Erica J.; Rothbaum, Barbara O.; Bradley, Bekh (2011). "Versatility of fear-potentiated startle paradigms for assessing human conditioned fear extinction and return of fear". Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience. 5: 77. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2011.00077. PMC  3221285. PMID  22125516.
  21. ^ Grillon, C.; Ameli, R.; Woods, S. W.; Merikangas, K.; Davis, M. (1991). "Fear-potentiated startle in humans: Effects of anticipatory anxiety on the acoustic blink reflex". Psychophysiology. 28 (5): 588–595. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1991.tb01999.x. PMID  1758934.
  22. ^ Norrholm, S.; Jovanovic, Tanja (2011). "Translational fear inhibition models as indices of trauma-related psychopathology". Current Psychiatry Reviews. 7 (3): 194–204. doi: 10.2174/157340011797183193.
  23. ^ Norrholm, S. D.; Jovanovic, T.; Olin, I. W.; Sands, L.; Karapanou, I.; Bradley, B.; Ressler, K. J. (2010). "Fear extinction in traumatized civilians with posttraumatic stress disorder: relation to symptom severity". Biological Psychiatry. 69 (6): 556–563. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.09.013. PMC  3052965. PMID  21035787.
  24. ^ Lang, P. J.; McTeague, L. M. (2009). "The anxiety disorder spectrum: Fear imagery, physiological reactivity, and differential diagnosis". Anxiety, Stress & Coping. 22 (1): 5–25. doi: 10.1080/10615800802478247. PMC  2766521. PMID  19096959.
  25. ^ a b Cousens, G.A.; Kearns, A.; Laterza, F.; Tundidor, J. (2012). "Excitotoxic lesions of the medial amygdala attenuate olfactory fear-potentiated startle and conditioned freezing behavior". Behavioural Brain Research. 229 (2): 427–432. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2012.01.011. PMID  22249137. S2CID  25116098.
  26. ^ Jovanovic, T; Norrholm, Seth D.; Fennell, Jennifer E.; Keyes, Megan; Fiallos, Ana M.; Myers, Karyn M.; Davis, Michael; Duncan, Erica J. (15 May 2009). "Posttraumatic stress disorder may be associated with impaired fear inhibition: relation to symptom severity". Psychiatry Research. 167 (1–2): 151–160. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2007.12.014. PMC  2713500. PMID  19345420.

Videos

Youtube | Vimeo | Bing

Websites

Google | Yahoo | Bing

Encyclopedia

Google | Yahoo | Bing

Facebook